Hicks Law?
Reaction Time In Combat
Modern Research Challenges The Martial
Training Sales Pitch!
By W. Hock Hochheim
Remember when, "be there is a second!" meant really fast? Or the common expression "it'll take half-a second."
Half a second! Man that is really fast, isn't it? There are 1,000
milliseconds in on second. So a half a second is 500 milliseconds. Just
think about how fast a second is, then envision half a second? We have
split the atom and we have split the second.
In the fast paced
world of super science and equations, experts needed to split the second
into 1,000 parts. For almost all normal activity slitting seconds in
tenths, quarters and halves is just a trial pursuit question, unless you
are worried about the finish line of NASCAR races, Olympic athletes or
the Kentucky Derby.
How fast is fast?
And, how much faster can we get? And how many changing variables are
there interfering with your speed? When did a split-second, or
half-a-second become slow-motion? These are also questions that come up
also in martial arts marketing and sales pitches. I will dissect the
misunderstood 1952 Hicks Law, and its twisted use in dumbing down
training problems.
Action beats reaction. If
you are reacting to an attack, as the good guys generally are, you are
already behind the action curve. How behind, scientists have labored
intensely to discover over the last 50 years, and like splitting the
atom, they have split the single second into one thousand parts to do
it.
It was about 25 years ago when I
attended a police defensive tactics course and was rather insulted by
the attitude of the instructor. We were treated like Neanderthals. He
declared, “KISS! Keep it simple, stupid. Hick's Law
says that it takes your mind too long to choose between two tactics.
Worse with three! Therefore, I will show you one response." I wondered then and there, "Am I to stay simple and stupid my whole life? Who is this Hick and what is his law?"
And, it takes too long?
How long was long? How long is TOO long? I wondered? We learned one
block versus a high punch that day. What about against a low punch, I
thought? My one high block fails to cover much else but that one high
attack.
Later that evening while
coaching my son's little league baseball team, I saw this very
instructor coaching his boy's team on another ball field. He was
teaching ten year-olds to multi-task and make split-second decisions as
his infielders, worked double plays with runners on base. It was clear
the coach expected more from these kids than he did from we adult cops
that morning. Hick's Law was not to be found on that kid's diamond.
Next, I slid both feet into this
thing called Hick's Law, to discover it was a growing favorite among
law enforcement trainers. Other famous police trainers kept mentioning
Hick's Law :
“ - lag time increases significantly with the greater number of techniques.”
" - it takes 58% more time to pick between two choices."
“ - it takes 'about a second' to pick a tactic.”
“Selection time gets compounded exponentially when a person has to select from several choices... ”
What is the
definition of "significant time?" 58% of what? What exactly is "about a
second?" Exponentially? Compounded? I had to delve even deeper into
these cavalier statements. They seemed to have an agenda. The agenda was
to sell training courses and dumb-down people and training? If I was
going to become this pessimistic, I needed more proof. I hit the
textbooks and contacted the experts.
The actual Hick's idea was based on a computer study, a paper
written in 1952 and simply set up an equation that states it takes time
to decide between options. Just for the record, the equation is TR+a+b{Log2 (N)}.
A computer performance study? Do you think that 1950 computers ran a
bit slow? The 1950 idea was then extrapolated into human performance,
usually based on very primitive, 1950 see-then-push-button tests. The
lab method had the testee selecting from several buttons on sudden
command. Somehow from this alone, the mythology of the slow brain, the
slow, stuttering, decision-making brain premise developed into a
combatives training doctrine.
Exponentially decision making?
Instructors often ignorantly tag Hicks law with exponential
math. Any exponential function is a constant multiple of its own
derivative. Many still just blindly associate a never-ending doubling ratio
to Hick - that is, for every two choices, selection time doubles per
added choice. Yet, despite all these quotes on times, Hick made no
official proclamation on the milliseconds it takes to mentally decide
between option choices. Meanwhile, experts say that logarithm math
actually relates to Hicks.
There is a general, consensus in the modern Kinesiology community that Simple Reaction Time, called SRT,
takes an average of 100 or 150 milliseconds to decide to take any
action. That's considerably less than a quarter of a second-or 250
milliseconds, or a 500 millisecond "half-a-second," or the loose "about a
second" we hear from martial trainers. Lets re-establish that there are
1,000 milliseconds in one second-a fact that makes all these time
studies fall to include into a proper perspective. 1,000 of them! More
than 1,000 milliseconds passed before you can read the first number in
that number aloud.
Based on the doubling/exponentially rule
with the commonly discussed SRT average, then choosing between two
choices must take 300 milliseconds. Run out that time-table. Three
choices? 600 milliseconds. Four choices? 1 second and 200 milliseconds. A
mere five choices? 2 seconds and 400 milliseconds! Six? 4 full seconds
and 800 milliseconds. Should a boxer learn 5 tactics? That would mean 9
seconds and 600 milliseconds to choose one tactic from another? You
would really see people physically shut down while trying to select
options at this point and beyond. Has this been your viewing experience
of a football game? Basketball? Tennis? Has this been your experience as
a witness to life? Under this casual, exponential increase rule, it
would seem athletes would stand dumbfounded, as index cards rolled
through their heads in an attempt to pick a choice of action. Every eye
jab could not be blocked if the blocker was taught even just two blocks.
The eye attack would hit the eyes as the defender sluggishly selects
between the two blocks.
One then begins to wonder how a
football game can be played, how a jazz pianist functions, or how a
bicyclist can pedal himself in a New York City rush hour. How does a
boxer, who sees a spilt-second opening, select a jab, cross, hook,
uppercut, overhand combination or to step back straight, right or left?
If he dares to throw combination punches how can he select them so
quickly?
Simple, modern athletic performance studies attack
the simplistic, doubling rule, but we need not only look to athletes.
How can a typist type so quickly? Look at all the selections on a
computer? 26 letters-plus options! How can you read this typed essay?
How can your mind select and process from 26 different letters in the
alphabet and spell with speed? How can an elderly person drive a car
across town? A child play soccer? It is obvious that the exponential
rule of “doubling” with each option, has serious scientific problems
when you run a simple math table out, or just look about you at everyday
life. And, despite the constant use of the word exponentially by
quoters, real experts clarify that logarithms should be used.
Exponential or logarithms the math of many choices do not play in the
life we see around us.
New
tests upon new tests on skills like driving vehicles, flying, sports and
psychology, have created so many layers of fresh information. Larish
and Stelmach in 1982 established that one could select from 20 complex
options in 340 milliseconds, providing the complex choices have been
previously trained. One other study even had a reaction time of .03
milliseconds between two trained choices! .03! Merkel's Law, for
example, says that trouble begins when a person has to select between 8
choices, but can still select a choice from the eight well under 500
milliseconds. Brace yourself! Mowbray and Rhoades Law of 1959, or the
Welford Law of 1986, even found no difference in reaction time at all,
when selecting from numerous, well-trained choices.
Why all these time differences?
Sometimes experts challenge test results by questioning the test
process and equipment involved. In 2003, I conducted an email survey of
50 college university professors of Psychology and Kinesiology. It is
crystal clear that training makes a considerable difference in reaction
time. Plus, people, tests and testing equipment are different.
Respondents state that every person and the skills they perform in tests
vary, so reaction times vary. One universal difficulty mentioned by
researchers is the mechanical task of splitting the second in their
testing - that is identifying the exact millisecond that the tested
reaction took place. Many recorded tests are performed by under-grads in
less than favorable conditions.
The test-givers themselves have
reaction time issues that effect time recording in their tests. When
milliseconds count, milliseconds can be wasted as the tester sees the
testee react, then reacts with a stopwatch device, either estimating or
losing milliseconds in their own reaction process. Common test machinery
takes milliseconds to register a choice. Results can get vague and
slippery within the tiny world of a single second. Documenting
milliseconds in the 1950s was primitive compared to modern,
sophisticated labs.
The KISS Method- not well thought out as a doctrine.
Many unintended messages and consequences are involved.
Discoveries made in 1990s,
decades after the 1950s Hicks law began, blowing the original,
antiquated "mental rolodex/task selection" concept out of the water as
an important martial training tenet. The brain has a fast track! Below,
researchers Martin D. Topper, Ph.D., and Jack M. Feldman, Ph.D. write
about them:
"Currently, the best explanation is provided by psychologist Gary Klein in Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions.
He's proposed that the human brain is capable of multi-tasking. Gary's
theory works like this: A visual image is picked up by the retina and is
transmitted to the visual center of the brain in the occipital lobe.
From there the image is sent to two locations in the brain. On the one
hand, it goes to the higher levels of the cerebral cortex which is the
seat of full conscious awareness. There, in the frontal lobes, the image
is available to be recognized, analyzed, input into a decision process
and acted upon as the person considers appropriate. Let's call this "the
slow track," because full recognition of the meaning of a visual image,
analyzing what it represents, deciding what to do and then doing it
takes time. Some psychologists also refer to this mental process as
System II cognition. If you used System II cognition in critical
situations like a skid, you wouldn't have enough time to finish
processing the OODA Loop before your car went over the cliff.
Fortunately, there's a second track, which we'll call "the fast track," or System I Cognition. In this system, the image is also sent to a lower, pre-conscious region of the brain, which is the amygdala. This area of the brain stores visual memory and performs other mental operations as well. The visual image is compared here on a pre-conscious level at incredible speed with many thousands of images that are stored in memory. Let's call each image a "frame" which is a term that Dr. Erving Goffman used in his book Frame Analysis to describe specific, cognitively-bounded sets of environmental conditions. I like to use the word "frame" here because the memory probably contains more than just visual information. There may be sound, kinesthetic, tactile, olfactory or other sensory information that also helps complement the visual image contained within the frame - fortunately, the fast and slow tracks are usually complimentary, one focusing on insight, the other on action. Together they produce a synergistic effect that enhances the actor's chances of survival.
But even though these two tracks are complimentary, we know that some people seem to be much more skilled than others at integrating System 1 and System 2. These especially competent individuals seem to resolve critical situations and also adapt to rapid changes in those situations. They invent routines they have never before performed and act in a fluid, seamless manner without employing full focal awareness."
So at this point in our understanding, we have newer models discovered and developing that tell us something about how the brain can operate on two tracks at the same time, but we don't really have a good idea of how the two levels interact, except to say that the interaction is very fast and complex, and some people do it better than others. We really don't know everything we'd like to know. But we do know that specific types of training can help a person develop unconscious competence, and this is enough to make some suggestions about the kind of training that will help make relatively unskilled people more competent in finding solutions to potentially violent encounters.
Fortunately, there's a second track, which we'll call "the fast track," or System I Cognition. In this system, the image is also sent to a lower, pre-conscious region of the brain, which is the amygdala. This area of the brain stores visual memory and performs other mental operations as well. The visual image is compared here on a pre-conscious level at incredible speed with many thousands of images that are stored in memory. Let's call each image a "frame" which is a term that Dr. Erving Goffman used in his book Frame Analysis to describe specific, cognitively-bounded sets of environmental conditions. I like to use the word "frame" here because the memory probably contains more than just visual information. There may be sound, kinesthetic, tactile, olfactory or other sensory information that also helps complement the visual image contained within the frame - fortunately, the fast and slow tracks are usually complimentary, one focusing on insight, the other on action. Together they produce a synergistic effect that enhances the actor's chances of survival.
But even though these two tracks are complimentary, we know that some people seem to be much more skilled than others at integrating System 1 and System 2. These especially competent individuals seem to resolve critical situations and also adapt to rapid changes in those situations. They invent routines they have never before performed and act in a fluid, seamless manner without employing full focal awareness."
So at this point in our understanding, we have newer models discovered and developing that tell us something about how the brain can operate on two tracks at the same time, but we don't really have a good idea of how the two levels interact, except to say that the interaction is very fast and complex, and some people do it better than others. We really don't know everything we'd like to know. But we do know that specific types of training can help a person develop unconscious competence, and this is enough to make some suggestions about the kind of training that will help make relatively unskilled people more competent in finding solutions to potentially violent encounters.
And then this news on BDNF: Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor :
"If I had to make a signal that could write messages on the brain from the environment, that would be BDNF."
Scientists
at Johns Hopkins and the National Cancer Institute have found a "missing
link" brain chemical that rises and falls quickly in response to
stress, fear or an upbeat mood, and then sculpts nerve circuits in the
brain accordingly. Their report, on work done appears in the Dec. 21,
1999 issue of the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
(PNAS). Further, because research at Hopkins and elsewhere shows that
BDNF levels vary with subject's experience as it goes down in stressful
situations..."BDNF has all the right features to be
the critical signal by which environmental and psychosocial
interactions impact on the brain," says neuropathologist Dr. Vassilis E.
Koliatsos. "It's very rapid, it's sensitive, and it affects a system
critical for emotional life and behavior. "What
we believe we've found is a link between what happens to a person on a
daily basis and the way the brain responds, from an emotional
standpoint, over the long term."
Dr Susan Greenfield has written The Quest For Identity In The 21st Century, in which she discusses the natural ways the human brain grows and adapts. "
I'm a neuroscientist and my day-to-day research at Oxford University
strives for an ever greater understanding - and therefore maybe, one
day, a cure - for Alzheimer's disease. But one vital fact I have learnt
is that the brain is not the unchanging organ that we once imagined. It
not only goes on developing, changing and, in some tragic cases,
eventually deteriorating with age, it is also substantially shaped by
what we do to it and by the experience of daily life. When I say
"shaped," I'm not talking figuratively or metaphorically; I'm talking
literally. At a microcellular level, the infinitely complex network of
nerve cells that make up the constituent parts of the brain actually
change in response to certain experiences and stimuli. The brain, in
other words, is malleable. The surrounding environment has a huge impact
both on the way our brains develop and how that brain is transformed
into a unique human mind.
Doctors
Richard A. Schmidt (a decades long expert) and Timothy Donald Lee, in
the , ground breaking, 1980s book and subsequent new editions since, Motor Control and Learning
reported that task selection is made up of two parts, RT (reaction
time) - seeing the problem, and MT (movement time) - physically moving
to respond, and thus may be a "few milliseconds " for fast, simple
chores, not this compounding, exponential, doubling, half-second and
full second formats.
And
another major factor, so simply explained in a sentence or two concerns
"arousal." Arousal is another word for alertness and also adrenaline in
performance sports and psychology. "One of the most investigated
factors affecting reaction time is 'arousal' or state of attention,
including muscular tension. Reaction time is fastest with an
intermediate level of arousal, and deteriorates when the subject is
either too relaxed or too tense (Welford, 1980; Broadbent, 1971;
Freeman, 1933)."
Practice helps. Dr. Robert J. Kosinski of Clemson University reported on his research in September of 2010 - "Sanders
(1998, p. 21) cited studies showing that when subjects are new to a
reaction time task, their reaction times are less consistent than when
they've had an adequate amount of practice. Ando et al. (2002)
found that reaction time to a visual stimulus decreased with three weeks
of practice, and the same research team (2004) reported that the
effects of practice last for at least three weeks. Fontani et al. (2006)
showed that in karate, more experienced practitioners had shorter
reaction times.... Visser et al. (2007)"
Nine decades of performance testing
and technology have passed since Hicks simple, little "Computer Choice
Law", with new technology and testing on athletes as well as regular,
everyday people. Not only are the testing methods better, and the
understanding superior, so are the new methodologies created to increase
SRT and selection times. Perhaps no better better statement damning the
Hicks law model as a foundation in physical training can be found than
from neuroplastician Dr. Michael Merzenich, regarded among experts as a
leading source on the human brain when reporting in the book, The Brain that Changes Itself, "we
can change the very structure of the brain and increase its
capacity...unlike a computer, the brain is constantly adapting itself."
In 2012, in the new book, Wait, the Art and Science of Delay,
Professor Frank Partnoy collects numerous studies on the split second,
or millisecond-second decision making of mental and physical choices. He
has all the very latest, 2012, medical and psychological testing on
sports, self defense on down to fast-paced, internet stock trading. It
is interesting to note that the infamous In this new book, Hicks Law is
not even mentioned, not a whisper. That is how research has advanced
from the 1950s.
In many ways Wait refutes a former bestseller, Malcolm Gladwell's Blink
by proving that the very best-of-the-best performers know how to delay
reaction to the last - well - millisecond, making the best choice. The
secret? Some genetics and a lot of proper training. Blink tells the reader to go with your first impulse. Wait tells
you to go with your last impulse. All these choices occur in less than a
second anyway and the book makes for good reading. It breaks down the
three critical steps - vision, decision and reaction averages, all in
the milliseconds arena with the latest, high-technology and knowledge.
About 100 milliseconds to see, about 200 milliseconds to decide what to
do among several choices, and about 200 milliseconds to action. About
half a second. (and this does not increase exponentially or in a
logarithm with multiple choices.)
How can we possible improve reaction times?
Aside
from the fact that Hicks Law exists in a world of 1,000 milliseconds
within one single second, here are some proven methods that improve
overall reaction time:
* Sequential Learning - the stringing of tasks working together like connected notes in music, really reduces reaction and selection time.
* Conceptual Learning -
is another speed track. In relation to survival training, this means a
person first makes an either/or conceptual decision, like “Shoot/Don't
shoot,” or, “Move-In/Move Back.” Rather than selecting from a series of
hand strikes, in Conceptual Learning, the boxer does not waste
milliseconds selecting specific punches, but rather makes one overall
decision, “punch many times!” The trained body then takes over,
following paths learned from prior repetition training.
* Implicit and Procedural Memory - Misinformed
proponents of Hicks Law would have you believe that people are forever
stumbling buffoons when given three or more options to choose from. Yet,
In Dr. Lee Dye's 2009 article for ABC News, "How the Brain Makes Quick
Decisions,” he reports: "(People)
…have been helped by a kind of human memory that scientists have been
struggling to understand” Dye reports that people use "implicit" memory,
a short-term memory that people are not consciously aware they are
using. Doctors Ken Paller at Northwestern University in Evanston, Ill.,
Joel L. Voss, from the Beckman Institute and the University of Illinois
at Urbana-Champaign have conducted long-term research on this subject
and while they did not specifically involve athletics, the conclusions
are consistent with other researchers who are also studying how top
athletes can make split-second decisions and take actions. How does a
batter hit a fastball when he has to start swinging the bat before the
ball even leaves the pitcher's hand? “He relies on visual cues, even if
he doesn't know it.” Athletes and people learn to predict and act, and
react spontaneously based on very little information. One way is
implicit memory.
Implicit memory (IM) is a type of
memory in which previous experiences aid in the performance of a task
without conscious awareness of these previous experiences. People rely
on implicit memory in a form called procedural memory - the type of
memory that allows people to remember how to tie their shoes or ride a
bicycle without consciously thinking about these activities. Implicit
memory taps into procedural memory.
One more definition in this chain
of memory and performance. Procedural memory. Connecting small,
multi-tasks and problem solving. Examples of procedural learning are
learning to ride a bike, learning to touch-type, learning to play a
musical instrument or learning to swim as well as performing athletic
tasks like sports. For our readers here this includes martial moves,
fighting, self defense and combatives. Experts report that procedural
memory can be very durable, however perishable like any task. And, the
physical fitness to perform these tasks may not be so durable. Given the
ravages of aging, a pro tennis player away from the game for many
years, is still likely to pick up a tennis racket and beat most common
tennis players, but not Wimbleton.
The Good, The Bad and The Simple
Sure, sure, sure, simple is good.
I am all for simple. Absolutely. And reaction time is an important
concern when you are dodging a knife, pulling a gun, etc. And there may
actually come a point in a learning progression when there are way, way
too many reactions/techniques to counter an attack, and If these moves
are a bit unnatural, and not guided somewhat by some natural reflex, and
taught poorly and out of context, a long list of untrained movements may cause performance problems. Poor systems and poor training may lead to untimely confusion. But we are not as simple and slow as Hick's Law misleaders want to scare us.
It seems like the last 8
decades, Hick's Legacy should really be telling us to train more and
smarter, not necessarily to be stupid and learn less. Remember one of
Einstein's Laws apply also - “Keep it simple…but not too simple.” I like the sound of that much better than stupid instructors KISSING me to keep things stupid. And still we learn more.
I report the following
information to remind us that we are not the slugs of Hicks law, or
slimed into slow motion in the world of milliseconds, or trapped within a
brain that runs like a 1950's morse code program. Take a moment to
renew confidence and examine new, 21st Century discoveries of our
brain...
Dr. M. Blackspear of the Brain Dynamics Center at the University of Sydney Australia reports that the: "...study
of functional inter-dependences between brain regions is a rapidly
growing focus of neuroscience research. This endeavor has been greatly
facilitated by the appearance of a number of innovative methodologies
for the examination of neurophysiological and neuroimaging data." This
Blackspear statement was made about the amazing new discoveries in 2005
and of how fast, repeat HOW FAST the healthy, human brain changes and
adapts "on the fly" - which is the new medical, catch phrase for such
studies on this now.. People select and change options "mid-flight" in
milliseconds split into milliseconds.
Intelligence matters as a variable. "The link between intelligence and reaction time is reviewed in Deary et al.
(2001). Serious mental retardation produces slower and more variable
reaction times. Among people of normal intelligence, there is a slight
tendency for more intelligent people to have faster reaction times, but
there is much variation between people of similar intelligence"
(Nettelbeck, 1980). The speed advantage of more intelligent people is
greatest on tests requiring complex responses (Schweitzer, 2001). This
study alone destroys the 60 year-old Hicks law.
"Stimulus–response
compatibility" is known to also affect reaction time for the Hick's
Law. This means that the response should be similar to the stimulus
itself. For example, turning a wheel to turn the wheels of the car is
good stimulus–response compatibility. The action the user performs is
similar to the response the driver receives from the car. Hitting a red
button when another red button comes on. Similarity. Familiarity.
Training creates this similarity and familiarity.
6 or more choices? 400 milliseconds to choose or 4 or even 6 seconds to rolodex through all of them? Remember the police trainer's quote of "about a second per choice?" Let's go back to the ol' ball game - and back to the baseball
analogy that started this article. We expect a common shortstop in
baseball to perform a select list of actions instantly at the crack of
the bat. The baseball shortstop is expected to:
- catch a ground ball to his left, or
- catch a ground ball to his center, or
- catch a ground ball straight at him, or
- catch a line drive, or
- catch a pop-up, or
- tag a runner out, or
- catch the ball traversing across second base for a double play, or
- instantly consider consequences to the overall game, like diving for the ball and missing
Moves all to be executed in the
sheer "splitest" of a split second? Then, our ape man, ball player has
even more split-second, follow-up decisions to make with runner's on
different bases. Even a child playing shortstop has a lot to decide and
very fast, AND can do it faster than 4 or 6 seconds or more! I hope that
the police trainer I mentioned in the beginning of this essay is
reading this and not just when he teaches his kids in little league, but
when he teaches his adults in law enforcement tactics. In fact, I hope
all martial instructors are listening?
In Summary
Recently in 2011, someone
accused me of claiming that Hick's Law doesn't exist and I was ignorant
of what Hick's Law really is. Of course it exists. A Mr. William Edmund
Hick existed. This British psychologist, Mr. Hicks created a test and
his test had results. The results were that response took time. The
central point of all this reaction research? Milliseconds. It is really about milliseconds.
Remember, there are 1,000 milliseconds in a second! Various studies
produce various millisecond results. All have improved on the Hicks
1950s response times, yet trainers use this law because of their
"dumb-down" agendas or they are ignorant and just regurgitate other
trainers.
Probably the single reason
Hick's Law has been spread in the last few decades in the police,
martial and military fields is as a sales pitch to sell training
programs. But, just how fast can we get? How dumb should we be to fight
back confusion and stalling out? Don't ask Mr. Hick from the 1950s. Mr.
Hick was not conducting tests on baseball or fighting, and the 1950's
computer he used belongs in the stone age of museum pieces.
Much more workable!
1) Hicks Law exists.
2) It is based on splitting milliseconds. There are 1,000 milliseconds within one second. Not many know this.
2) It is based on splitting milliseconds. There are 1,000 milliseconds within one second. Not many know this.
3) There are other, more modern reaction studies with differing and even faster results than the old Hicks study.
4) Hicks law is misused. It is misunderstood. It is blindly regurgitated.
5) The misuses and misunderstandings are used to sell training programs, or to feign a certain expertise.
6) It is frequently used to dumb-down police, military and martial arts programs.
7) People can only get so fast within these milliseconds anyway.
8) Hicks widely accepted version of math and expanding delays between multiple choices cannot be played out in the reality we witness in our daily lives around us.
9) Many other definable issues can cause choice-delay and all delays cannot be blamed on Hicks Law.
5) The misuses and misunderstandings are used to sell training programs, or to feign a certain expertise.
6) It is frequently used to dumb-down police, military and martial arts programs.
7) People can only get so fast within these milliseconds anyway.
8) Hicks widely accepted version of math and expanding delays between multiple choices cannot be played out in the reality we witness in our daily lives around us.
9) Many other definable issues can cause choice-delay and all delays cannot be blamed on Hicks Law.
10) Hicks Law and it's milliseconds are virtually inconsequential as a martial training tenet.
*****
An example of a famous instructor misusing Hick's Law - See the 23 April entry click here
Read more! Brain Size and Intelligence? Fact of Fiction- Click
Got an opinion on this one? Or new research? We'd like to hear it and add it here. Post it at HocksCombatForum. Click here
Return to master article page click here
Another book to read on this subject...
Thinking Fast and Slow By Daniel Kahneman
So much training
mythology (notice I did not say "methodology") concerns itself with
choice selection under stress, and as a result the 60-year-old "Hicks
Law" has been regurgitated over and over again, rather mindlessly, as a
reason not to learn too many things. Hicks is a very broad, misleading
and ill-informed law causing a general dumbing-down" of individual
potential. Here's a book that inspects the thinking and decision process
in very big way, immaculately researched by this expert - the author
himself. This is NOT a book about fighting, though police, military and
firefighters are often referenced. This is about about thinking fast and
slow, and it shoves the abstract, Hicks law further and further over
into the dark corner where it belongs.
"Hi Hock, A friend forwarded
me your article on Hick's Law, great job of digging into the concept.
Too often, concepts like this from kinesiology or motor control are
taken at face value and made into dogma. Motor learning and control is a
key area of interest for me; I teach courses in Health and Human
Performance (what used to just be called PE) and got into the field
because of my interest motor control as a martial artist. The more we
can apply those concepts to our training, the better our training will
be. I enjoy your work and resources, thanks!" - Randy Simpson M.S.
C.P.T."
*****
W. Hock Hochheim is a military
and police vet with multiple black belts. he currently teaches
practical, tactical hand, stick, knife and gun tactics and strategies in
11 allied countries each year. He can be reached through www.HocksCQC.com
No comments:
Post a Comment